Past tense experiments

Mark McConville
Henry S. Thompson
8 March 2012
Creative CommonsAttributionShare Alike

1. Regular and irregular verbs in the Brown corpus

The ten most commonly occurring verbs in the million-word Brown Corpus:

Note that all of these are irregular

The first ten least commonly occurring verbs in the Brown Corpus:

Note that all of these are regular.

Of the 877 verbs which occur just once in the Brown Corpus:

Rarity appears to hurt irregular verbs, but not regular ones.

2. Memory and irregular verbs

Basic property of memory - the more often you hear something, the better you remember it

Irregular verbs are the most common in English (as well as in other languages)

Old English had three times as many strong verbs as Modern English

Joan Bybee studied 33 Old English strong verbs that survive in Modern English:

3. Rare irregulars, cont'd

Some rare irregular past tense forms are sliding out of English as we speak

In many cases, the unnaturalness is relevant only to the past tense form, rather than to the verb itself

Irregular past tense verbs can part company from their stems, and accrue different degrees of familiarity

4. Naturalness of past tense verb forms

Michael Ullman and Steven Pinker investigated the "gut reactions" of 99 adult English speakers to different verbs, including past tense forms.

Participants were asked to rate the naturalness of different verb forms, on a scale of 1 (unnatural) to 7 (natural).

Verb stems and past tense forms were judged separately, to distinguish between

For irregular past tense verb forms, the rating depended on the frequency of the past tense forms themselves in the language

For regular past tense forms, the rating was independent of the frequency of the past tense forms in the language

These results support the hypothesis that the mind handles regular and irregular past tense forms differently

5. Language production experiments

Sandeep Prasada, William Snyder and Steven Pinker investigated how quickly English speakers could produce past tense forms.

Participants sat at a computer, had verb stems flashed at them, and had to say the relevant past tense form as quickly as they could.

A voice-operated trigger was used to time exactly how long it took them to read the stem, mentally compute the past tense form, and say it out loud.

Again, these results support the hypothesis that irregular past tense forms are stored in memory but regular past tense forms aren't.

These results have been replicated consistently by other teams of researchers.

6. Word recognition experiments

Lexical decision tasks - participants see or hear a sequence of real words and fake words (e.g. "narse" or "bluck") and have to press one button for a real word and a different button for a fake word.

Lexical decision tasks tell us something about how the mental lexicon is organised.

Repetition priming - if particiants are given a word, and then a short time later are given it again, they are faster at recognising it second time around.

The priming effect also extends to related words, e.g. from "doctor" to "nurse", or from "duck" to "goose".

7. Priming word recognition

Robert Stanners investigated using past tense forms to prime verb stems

These results suggest that the lexical entry retrieved when recognising a regular past tense verb form is the corresponding verb stem entry itself.

On the other hand, the entry for an irregular past tense form is separate from, but hot-linked to, that of its verb stem.

Again these results have been reproduced many times, including in experimental settings where brain activity is measured directly, by electrodes pasted to the scalp.

Note that it is not the case that the priming effect is caused by mere phonological overlap

8. Cross-modal priming

Can we use the acoustic form of a word to prime its written representation?

For example, experimental participants in lexical decision tests hear some words and see others on the computer screen.

These examples of cross-modal priming provide evidence that priming occurs deep within the mind, rather at the shallow levels of perception.

William Marslen-Wilson and Lorraine Tyler investigated cross-modal priming with regular and irregular past tense verb forms.

These results were confirmed using subliminal priming

Marslen-Wilson and Tyler also showed that the associative link between an irregular past tense form (e.g. "gave") and its stem (i.e. "give") is stronger than those between semantically related words like "duck" and "goose"

9. Recap: the words-and-rules model

The simple words-and-rules model proposes that:

The blocking principle is used to resolve potential conflicts

This is a dual mechanism model of cognition, since it presupposes two completely different kinds of "mental tissue":

Together, the two mechanisms give rise to system which is both:

10. Recap: the words-and-rules model - evaluation

The words-and-rules model provides an explanation for the amazing productivity of the regular past:

However, the basic words-and-rules model cannot explain observations relating to the patterns found among the irregular verbs:

The patterns found among the irregular verbs are not just of etymological interest - they appear to be active (in some way) inside the minds of present-day English speakers.

11. Recap: the SPE model

Chomsky & Halle present a single mechanism model of past tense inflection:

However, SPE was never meant to be taken as a theory of how linguistic knowledge is stored in the brain, put to use in language production and understanding, or acquired by children.

12. Recap: the connectionist model

Connectionist models (e.g. Rumelhart and MacLelland's) present another single mechanism model of past tense inflection.

Regular and irregular past tense morphology are again handled in the same way

Connectionist models are very good at learning the patterns inherent in irregular past tense morphology.

And given just the right mixtures of regular and irregular verbs during particular phases of training, they can be made to mimick the U-shaped curve of child language acquisition.

13. Recap: the connectionist model - downsides

However, even the most sophisticated connectionist models of English past tense inflection exhibit much lower accuracy with regular verbs.

This is fundamentally because sound similarity is not an important feature for regular verbs in English:

No connectionist model has been able to successfully learn the default nature of the regular past inflection.

Pattern associator memories cannot exploit variables - the basic gadget of computation.

14. The augmented words-and-rules model

Pinker proposes an "augmented" version of the basic words-and-rules model:

But memory itself is not a list of unrelated slots (like computer RAM).

In associative memory, words are linked to other, similar words

In this kind of model, families of irregular verbs are easier to store and retrieve, since these verbs repeatedly strengthen their shared associations.

15. The augmented words-and-rules model (ctd.)

The augmented words-and-rules model combines the best bits of all the previous models

Together these two mechanisms provide an explanation for all the ways in which the mind appears to process irregular verb inflection differently to regular verb inflection.

They also provide an explanation for the characteristic U-shaped development when a child learns past tense morphology:

16. Neologisms

We've mentioned neologisms, i.e. new words entering the language.

Six kinds of derived word can never have irregular inflected forms, even if they resemble other irregular words phonologically.

1. Onomatopoeic words, i.e. those which are perceived to resemble sounds:

2. Quotations, i.e. "mentioned" words:

3. Names, i.e. words derived from proper names:

4. Foreign loanwords:

5. Abbreviations and truncations:

6. Derived words, i.e. converted from other parts-of-speech, e.g.:

17. A brief history of 'fly'

Pinker spends a lot of time discussing "fly" and "flied", so it's worth a little bit of time picking it apart:

18. Systematic regularisations

There are lots of other examples of irregular words that get systematically regularised when used in certain ways:

These can be explained through the interaction between words and rules.

The regular inflection rules step in here, not because the irregular forms cannot be retrieved from memory, but because the derived words themselves are not stored in the normal, "canonical" format.

19. Word structure theory

The systemic regularisations discussed above contrast with other examples of derived verbs that do take irregular past forms:

What is the difference between these two kinds of word formation?

Morphologists claim that a prefixed verb like "outfly" is both:

In other words, "outflying" is a particular kind of "flying".

However, denominal verbs like "fly (out)" do not have these two properties:

The same thing goes for the other examples of systematic regularisation:

This explanation depends on having a distinction between words and rules:

Lab experiments have shown that people do systematically regularise brand-new denominal verbs they have never heard before, even if they sound like normal irregular verbs:

But they don't regularise "semantically stretched" verbs in the same way:

20. Count nouns and mass nouns

English common nouns divide up into two main classes:

Count nouns all have plural forms, denoting a group of two or more of the relevant things

Mass nouns do not have plural forms:

Caveat 1: mass nouns can often be repackaged as count nouns

Caveat 2: count nouns can often be repackaged as mass nouns

Caveat 3: a few plural nouns don't have singular base forms:

21. Regular plurals in English

Regular plurals in English are remarkably similar to regular past tense forms.

A single suffix morpheme is realised using three distinct, phonologically conditioned allomorphs:

These three allomorphs can be captured by the usual phonological rules of anaptyxis and devoicing:

22. Irregular plurals in English

Seven commonly used English nouns form their plural by changing the internal vowel

Three nouns have kept the Anglo-Saxon plural suffix -en:

Some nouns denoting "gregarious animals that are hunted, gathered or farmed" are identical in the singular and plural

Some nouns voice the final [f], [θ] or [s] consonant of the stem, before adding the plural suffix:

Some "academic" nouns borrowed from Latin keep their original plural forms:

As do some borrowed from Greek:

But many other Latin and Greek nouns take normal regular plurals: